Linguistic and prosodic features of motherese and preterm infants' vocalizations during kangaroo care A microanalytical study
Main Article Content
Abstract
Motherese has been studied particularly in its prosodic features. The scientific literature has underlined the importance of this type of communication on the infants’ vocal responsiveness. However, we still know little about the role of motherese on preterm infants’ vocal responsiveness. We intend to know the prosodic and communicative characteristics of motherese in preterm dyads and particularly to understand its relationship with the preterm infant’s vocal responsiveness. At NICU, mothers (N = 38) were asked to speak and to sing without words (humming) to their preterm infants in kangaroo care during five periods of three minutes alternating voice and silence, controlling the order effect (silence – speech or humming – silence – humming or speech – silence). A microanalytical study about prosodic and communicative/affectionate features of motherese was performed using ELAN, MAXQDA, and PRAAT software. According to results, tonal contours (sinusoidal, U-shaped and falling) and infants’ vocalizations seem to contribute for preterm dyads’ vocal modulation. A high use of phatic and conative functions, interrogative utterances, infants’ positive aspects, infants designated by affectionate words, and utterances connected with infants' needs were observed. This study contributed to explore the role of the communicative/affectionate and prosodic features of the motherese on preterm infants’ vocal responsiveness during the kangaroo care in NICU. Still, more studies are needed to deepen these preliminary results.
Downloads
Article Details
An open-access CREATIVE COMMONS copyright license is used. Those authors whose works are published by this journal, accept the following terms:
- Authors will retain their copyright and guarantee the Journal the right to first publish their work, which will simultaneously be subject to the Creative Commons Recognition License CC BY SA that allows third parties to share the work, provided that its author and first publication is indicated.
- Authors may adopt other non-exclusive license agreements for the distribution of the published version of the work (e.g., deposit it in an institutional telematics file or publish it in a monographic volume) provided that the initial publication in this journal is indicated.
- Authors are allowed and recommended to disseminate their work over the Internet (e.g. in institutional telematics files or on their website) before and during the submission process, which can produce interesting exchanges and increase quotes of the published work.
References
Bartha-Doering, Lisa, Johanna Alexopoulos, Vito Giordano, Lisa Stelzer, Theresa Kainz, Silvia Benavides-Varela, Isabell Wartenburger, Katrin Klebermass-Schrehof, Monika Olischar, Rainer Seidl, and Angelika Berger. 2019. Absence of neural speech discrimination in preterm infants at term-equivalent age. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 39: 100679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100679
Bettes, Barbara. 1988. Maternal Depression and Motherese: Temporal and Intonational Features. Child Development, 59 (4): 1089-1096. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130275
Bozzette, Maryann. 2008. Healthy Preterm Infant Responses to Taped Maternal Voice. Journal of Perinatal and Neonatal Nursing, 22 (4): 307–316.
https://nursing.ceconnection.com/ovidfiles/00005237-200810000-00013.pdf
Brazelton, Thomas Berry, and Kevin Nugent. 2011. The Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale, 4th Edition. London: Mac Keith Press.
Buil, Aude, Isabelle Carchon, Giselle Apter, François Xavier Laborne, Maya Gratier, and Emmanuel Devouche. 2016. Kangaroo supported diagonal flexion positioning: New insights into skin-to-skin contact for communication between mothers and very preterm infants. Archives de Pédiatrie, 23 (9): 913–920.
¬https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arcped.2016.04.023
Carvalho, Maria Eduarda, João Manuel Justo, Maya Gratier, and Helena Maria Rodrigues. 2019a. The impact of maternal voice on the fetus: A systematic review. Current Women’s Health Reviews, 15 (3): 196–206. https://doi.org/10. 2174/15734048
Carvalho, Maria Eduarda, João Manuel Justo, Maya Gratier, Teresa Tomé, Esmeralda Pereira and Helena Rodrigues. 2019b. Vocal responsiveness of preterm infants to maternal infant-directed speaking and singing during skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) in the NICU. Infant Behavior and Development, 57: 101332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101332
Caskey, Melinda, Bonnie Stephens, Richard Tucker, and Betty Vohr. 2011. Importance of parent talk on the development of preterm infant vocalizations. Pediatrics, 128 (5): 910–916.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0609
Caskey, Melinda, Bonnie Stephens, Richard Tucker, and Betty Vohr. 2014. Adult talk in the NICU with preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics, 133 (3): e578–e584.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-0104
Cooper, Robin Panneton, Jane Abraham, Sheryl Berman and Margaret. 1997. The Development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20 (4): 477-488.
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638397900370
Correia, Luciana Leonetti, Ana Emília Vita Carvalho, and Maria Beatriz Martins Linhares. 2008. Verbal contents expressed by mothers of preterm infants with clinical emotional symptoms. Revista Latino-Americana de Enfermagem, 16 (1): 64-70. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-11692008000100011
Dominguez, Sarah., Emmanuel Devouche, Giselle Apter, and Maya Gratier. 2016. The roots of turn-taking in the neonatal period. Infant and Child Development, 25: 240–255. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1976
Dominguez, Sarah, Maya Gratier, Karine Martel, Aude Buil, Giselle Apter and Emmanuel Devouche. 2017. Le nouveau-né, un partenaire pour sa mère: Analyse du discours maternel. Neuropsychiatrie de l’Enfance et de l’Adolescence, 65 (4): 201-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurenf.2017.03.005
Dunbar, Robin. 1993. Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16 (4): 681-735.
https://www.uvm.edu/pdodds/files/papers/others/1993/dunbar1993a.pdf
Eckerman, Carol, Jerri Oehler, Mandy Medvin, and Thomas Hannan. 1994. Premature newborns as social partners before term age. Infant Behavior and Development, 17 (1): 55-70.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(94)90022-1
Falk, Simone. 2011. Melodic versus intonational coding of communicative functions: A comparison of tonal contours in infant-directed song and speech. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 21 (1-2): 54–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094004
Fellman, Vineta, Elena Kushnerenko, Kaija Mikkola, Rita Ceponiené, Jaana Leipälä, and Risto Näätänen. 2004. Atypical auditory event-related potentials in preterm infants during the first year of life: a possible sign of cognitive dysfunction? Pediatric Research, 56: 291–297.
https://doi.org/10.1203/01.PDR.0000132750.97066.B9
Fernald, Anne. 1989. Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development, 60 (6): 1497-1510. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130938
Fernald, Anne. 1991. Prosody in speech to children: Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Annals of Child Development, 8: 43-80.
Fernald, Anne, and Patricia Kuhl. 1987. Acoustic Determinants of Infant Preference for Motherese Speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 279-293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8
Filipe, Marisa, Sue Peppé, Sónia Frota, and Selene Vicente. 2017. Prosodic development in European Portuguese from childhood to adulthood. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38 (5): 1045-1070.
DOI:10.1017/S0142716417000030
Filippa, Manuela, Maya Gratier, Emmanuel Devouche, and Didier Grandjean. 2018. Changes in infant-directed speech and song are related to preterm infant facial expression in the neonatal intensive care unit.” Interaction Studies, 19 (3): 427–444. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.16019.fil
Filippa, Manuela, Constantino Panza, Fabrizio Ferrari, Rossella Frassoldati, Pierre Kuhn, Sara Balduzzi, and Roberto D’Amico. 2017. Systematic review of maternal voice interventions demonstrates increased stability in preterm infants. Acta Paediatrica, 106 (8): 1220-1229. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.13832
Filippa, Manuela, Damiano Menin, Roberta Panebianco, Maria Grazia Monaci, Marco Dondi, and Didier Grandjean. 2020. Live maternal speech and singing increase self-touch and eye-opening in preterm newborns: A preliminary study. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 44 (4): 453–473. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-020-00336-0
Filippa, Manuela, Elisa Della Casa, Roberto D’Amico, Odoardo Picciolini, Clara Lunardi, Alessandra Sansavini, and Fabrizio Ferrari. 2021. Effects of early vocal contact in the neonatal intensive care unit: Study protocol for a multi-centre, randomised clinical trial. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18 (8): Article 3915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083915
Fluharty, Melissa, Lynne S. Nemeth, Ayaba Logan and Michelle Nichols. 2021. What do neonatal intensive care unit policies tell us about kangaroo care implementation? A Realist Review. Advances in Neonatal Care, 21(4): E76–E85. https://doi.org/10.1097/ANC.0000000000000808
Frota, Sónia, Marisa Cruz, Nuno Matos, and Marina Vigário. 2016. Early Prosodic Development: Emerging intonation and phrasing in European Portuguese. In M. Armstrong, N. C. Henriksen, & M. M. Vanrell (Eds.), Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across linguistic subfields (pp. 295-324). Philadelphia, USA: John Benjamins. DOI: 10.1075/ihll.6.14fro
García-Sierra, Adrián, Nairán Ramírez-Esparza, Noelle Wig, and Dylan Robertson. 2021. Language learning as a function of infant directed speech (IDS) in Spanish: Testing neural commitment using the positive-MMR, Brain and Language, 212 : 104890.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2020.104890
Goldstein, Michael, and Jennifer Schwade. 2008. Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19 (5) : 515-523.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x
Gratier, Maya, Emmanuel Devouche, Bahia Guellai, Rubia Infanti, Ebru Yilmaz, and Erika Parlato-Oliveira. 2015. Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 1167. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167
Grieser, Dianne, and Patricia Kuhl. 1988. Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Developmental Psychology, 24 (1): 14-20.
http://ilabs.uw.edu/sites/default/files/1988%20Grieser%20_%20Kuhl.pdf
Hartman, Kelly, Nan Bernstein Ratner, and Rochelle Suzanne Newman. 2017. Infant directed speech (IDS), vowel clarity and child language outcomes. Journal Child Language, 44 (5): 1140-1162.
DOI: 10.1017/S0305000916000520
Jakobson, Roman. 1961. Linguistics and Communication Theory. Providence: American Mathematical Society.
Keren Miri, Ruth Feldman, Arthur Eidelman, Leah Sirota, and Barry Lester. 2003. Clinical Interview for high-risk parents of premature infants (CLIP) as a predictor of early disruptions in the mother-infant relationship at the nursery. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24: 93–110. DOI: 10.1002/imhj.10049
Kisilevsky, Barbara, Sylvia Hains, Cecilia Ann Brown, Charlotte Tsz Sum Lee, B. Cowperthwaite, Sherri Schmidt Stutzman, Melissa L. Swansburg, Lee Kang, Xing Xie, Hefeng Huang, Haihui Ye, Ke Zhang, and Zengping Wang. 2009. Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behavior and Development, 32 (1): 59-71. DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002
Kisilevsky, Barbara, and Sylvia Hains. 2011. Onset and maturation of fetal heart rate response to the mother’s voice over late gestation. Developmental Science, 14 (2): 214-223.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00970.x
Kisilevsky, Barbara, Beverly Chambers, Kevin Parker, and Gregory Davies. 2014. Auditory processing in growth-restricted fetuses and newborns and later language development. Clinical Psychology Sciences, 2 (4): 495-513. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/2167702613509371
Kitamura, Christine, and Denis Burnham (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother’s speech: adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy, 4 (1): 85–110.
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1207/S15327078IN0401_5
Kitamura, Christine, C. Thanavishuth, Denis K. Burnham, and Sudaporn Luksaneeyanawin. 2001. Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development, 24 (4): 372-392.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3
Kostilainen, Kaisamari, Eino Partanen, Kaija Mikkola, Valtteri Wikström, Satu Pakarinen, Vineta Fellman, and Minna Huotilainen. 2020. Neural processing of changes in phonetic and emotional speech sounds and tones in preterm infants at term age. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 148: 111–118. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.10.009
Kuhl, Patricia, Feng-Ming Tsao, and Huei-Mei Liu. 2003. Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100 (15): 9096-9101. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1532872100.
Lam-Cassettari, Christa, and Jane Kohlhoff. 2020. Effect of maternal depression on infant-directed speech to prelinguistic infants: Implications for language development. PloS one, 15 (7): e0236787. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236787
Landry, Susan, Karen Smith, Cynthia Miller-Loncar, and Paul Swank. 1997. Predicting cognitive-language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varied degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 33 (6): 1040-1053. DOI: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.1040
Lavelli, Manuela, and Alan Fogel. 2013. Interdyad differences in early mother–infant face-to-face communication: Real-time dynamics and developmental pathways. Developmental Psychology, 49 (12): 2257–2271. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032268
Lecanuet Jean-Pierre, Carolyn Granier-Deferre, Anthony DeCasper, Rosalyne Maugeais, André Jacques Andrieu, and Marie-Claire Busnel. 1987. Fetal perception and discrimination of speech stimuli; demonstration by cardiac reactivity; preliminary results. Comptes Rendus de l'Academie des Sciences. Série III, Sciences de la vie, 305 (5): 161-1644.
Macwhinney, Brian, and Catherine Snow. 1985. The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12: 271-296. DOI: 10.1017/S0305000900013866
Malloch, Stephen, and Colwyn Trevarthen (Eds.). 2009. Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Paavola, Leila, Sari Kunnari, Irma Moilanen, and Matti Lehtihalmes. 2005. The functions of maternal verbal responses to prelinguistic infants as predictors of early communicative and linguistic development. First Language, 25 (2): 173-195. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0142723705050341
Palazzi Ambra, Manuela Filippa, Rita Meschini, and Cesar Augusto Piccinini. 2021. Music therapy enhances preterm infant's signs of engagement and sustains maternal singing in the NICU. Infant Behavior and Development, 64: 101596. DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101596
Pan, Barbara Alexander, Alison Imbens-Bailey, Kendra Winner, and Catherine Snow. 1996. Communicative intents expressed by parents in interaction with young children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42: 248-266. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23087879
Papousek, Mechthild 1996. Intuitive parenting: a hidden source of musical stimulation in infancy. In I. Deliege, & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Musical beginnings. Origins and development of musical competence (pp. 88-112). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Papousek, Mechthild, Hanuš Papousek, and David Symmes. 1991. The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behavior and Development, 14 (4): 415-440.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(91)90031-M
Parlato-Oliveira, Erika, Catherine Saint-Georges, David Cohen, Hugues Pellerin, Isabella Marques Pereira, Catherine Fouillet, Mohamed Chetouani, Marc Dommergues, and Sylvie Viaux-Savelon. 2021. «Motherese» Prosody in Fetal-Directed Speech: An Exploratory Study Using Automatic Social Signal Processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 12: 646170. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646170
Pêcheux, Marie-Germaine, Florence Labrell, F., and Marc Pistorio. 1993. What do parents talk about to infants? Early Developmental Parent, 2 (2): 89–97.
Pelaez, Martha, Javier Virués-Ortega, and Jacob L. Gewirtz. 2011. Contingent and non-contingent reinforcement with maternal vocal imitation and motherese speech: Effects on infant vocalizations. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 12: 277–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2011.11434370
Peña, Marcela, Janet Werker, and Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz. 2012. Earlier speech exposure does not accelerate speech acquisition. Journal of Neuroscience, 32 (33): 11159-11163.
DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6516-11.2012.
Pessoa, Luciana, and Maria Lucia Seidl de Moura. 2008. Pragmatic characteristics of maternal speech in mother-child dyads (five and twenty months old). Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia, 60 (1): 82-95. https://pepsic.bvsalud.org/pdf/arbp/v60n1/v60n1a08.pdf
Pisanski, Katarzyna, Kavya Bhardwaj, and David Reby. 2018. Women's voice pitch lowers after pregnancy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39 (4): 457-463.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.04.002s
Rheingold, Harriet, and Judith Adams. 1980. The significance of speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 16 (5): 397-403.
Seidl de Moura, Maria Lucia, Adriana Ferreira Paes Ribas, Karla da Costa Seabra, Luciana Fontes Pessôa, Rodolfo de Castro Ribas Jr., and Susana Engelhard Nogueira. 2004. Interações iniciais mãe-bebê. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 17 (3): 295-302.
https://www.scielo.br/j/prc/a/BxBdxRTQjSW5tq94xcJbRNQ/?lang=pt&format=pdf
Rivero, Magda. 2010. Maternal expression of communicative intentions and pragmatic fine tuning in early infancy. Infant Behavior & Development, 33 (4): 373-386. DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.002
Shoemark, Helen, Marie Dahlstrøm, Oscar Bedford, and Lauren Stewart. 2021. The Effect of a Voice-Centered Psycho-Educational Program on Maternal Self-Efficacy: A Feasibility Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18 (5): 2537. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18052537
Snow, Catherine. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549-565.
Snow, Catherine. (1977). Development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4: 1-22.
Snow, Catherine, Barbara Alexander Pan, Alison Imbens-Bailey, and Jane Herman. 1996. Learning how to say what one means: a longitudinal study of children’s speech act use. Social Development, 5: 56-84.
Spence, Melanie, and Anthony DeCasper. 1987. Prenatal experience with low-frequency maternal-voice sounds influence neonatal perception of maternal voice samples. Infant Behavior and Development, 10 (2): 133-142.
Stern, Daniel, Susan Spieker, and Kristine MacKain. 1982. Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18 (5): 727–735.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.727
Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1993. The self-born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp.121–173). Cambridge University. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511664007.009
Van Egeren, Laurie, Marguerite Barratt, and Mary Roach. 2001. Mother–infant responsiveness: Timing, mutual regulation, and interactional context. Developmental Psychology, 37 (5): 684–697.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.37.5.684
Zelkowitz, Phyllis, Claudette Bardin, and Apostolos Papageorgiou. 2007. Anxiety affects the relationship between parents and their very low birth weight infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28: 296–313. DOI:10.1002/imhj.20137